Language scratchpad/Altlangs/Alternate reconstructions/Proto-Japonic scratchpad/Descendants

From Innocence Seekers
Jump to navigation Jump to search

This page compares some of the descendant conlangs of Proto-Japonic. So far, three languages are featured here: Namari (namaikotoba), New Kudara (ftārafča) and Tanmuran (tammurakotoba). Of the three, Namari is the one I've developed the most. In the Innocence Seekers setting, New Kudara (spoken in Kudara province in Namari) is the only surviving Peninsular Japonic language, while both Namari (spoken in Namari and Likkra) and Tanmuran (spoken on Jeju island in Korea and on Tsushima and Kyushu in Japan) are Insular Japonic languages.

Disclaimer: The entirety of this article's contents is to be considered science fiction. They are not, in any way, verified and accepted reconstructions of real-life proto-languages, even if they are largely based on accepted reconstructions.

Phonology

Vowels

Namari has by far the most vowels of any Japonic language in the Innocence Seekers setting, with between nine and eleven phonemic vowels depending on dialect (the most in any real-life Japonic language I know of is seven). On the other hand, Tanmuran only has five phonemic vowels (like Japanese), and New Kudara has six. New Kudara also has vowel harmony based on vowel height, with /a/, /e/ and /o/ forming one group and /ə/, /i/ and /u/ forming the other group.

The following table lists the reflexes of each Proto-Japonic vowel and vowel sequence:

Proto-Japonic Namari New Kudara Tanmuran
*i i (null), a, ə, (word-initial) s, (after /w/) i i
*e e, i i, e e, i
*u u (null), a, ə, (word-initial) f u
*o o, u u, o o, u
o o, u o
*a a a, ə a
*ei, *ui, *oi i (null), a, ə, (word-initial) s, (after /w/) i i
*əi e, i i, e (e, i after coronals) e, i
*ai, *iə, *ia e e, i e
*au, *ua o o, u o

Consonants

As few as nine consonants are reconstructed for Proto-Japonic. Voicing was likely allophonic, with obstruents voiced intervocally (this behaviour is still retained in Yonaguni, although the voiced consonants have become phonemic). What became the "muddy" consonants in Japanese (in most dialects, these consonants are plain voiced) were descended from prenasalised consonants, and the intervocalic obstruents lost their voicing (in most dialects; Tōhoku dialects do still retain intervocalic voicing).

If the prenasalised consonants are considered their own phonemes, this means that Proto-Japonic had thirteen consonant phonemes. The outcome of each consonant in the three languages featured is as followed:

Proto-Japonic Namari New Kudara Tanmuran
Default Before /i/ Before /u/ Default Before /i/ Before /u/ Default Before /i/ Before /u/
*k k k k k t͡ɕ f k k k
*nk g g g g ʑ v g g g
*s s ɕ s s ɕ s s ɕ s
*ns z ʑ z z ʑ z z ʑ z
*t t t͡ɕ θ t t͡ɕ t͡s t t͡ɕ t͡s
*nt d ʑ z d ʑ z d ʑ z
*n n ɲ n n z n n ɲ n
*p p p p p s f ɸ, h(a) ɸ ɸ
*mp b b b b z v b b b
*m m m m m n m m m m
*j j, (null)(e) N/A j j N/A i j, (null)(e) N/A j
*r j, (null)(e) (null) j r l r r r r
*w w(a), (null) (null) N/A w, (null)(o) w N/A w(a), (null) (null) N/A

While intervocalic */p/ is retained in New Kudara, both Namari and Tanmuran eventually lose it in most environments. In Namari, most instances of intervocalic */p/ became first */ɸ/, then /h/ in Middle Namari. The only exceptions were if they occurred after /u/, in which case the expected reflex is /w/. By the Modern Namari period, almost all /h/ were lost (the only exception being the topic marker), while instances of /w/ before /e/ and /i/ were subject to monophthongisation if they occurred after /u/; only /w/ before /a/ and after /u/ was retained.

Tanmuran underwent a development similar to that of Japanese and the Ryukyuan languages, namely, the unconditional change of intervocalic */p/ to /w/. This /w/ was eventually lost before all vowels except /a/.

Namari has the following consonant inventory:

Labial Alveolar Palatal Velar Glottal
Nasal m n ɲ (ŋ)
Stop p b t d k g
Fricative ɸ (β) θ (ð) ç x (ɣ) (h)
Sibilant s z ɕ ʑ
Affricate (t͡s) (d͡z) t͡ɕ (d͡ʑ)
Approximant j w
Liquid ɺ

Tanmuran has a similar consonant inventory, except that its /θ/ is an aspirated stop [tʰ] instead, and that it has a phonemic /h/ and /t͡s/. The difference between /t/ and /θ/ in Tanmuran is neutralised word-initially, with both being realised as [tʰ]. Namari /ɸ/ and /x/ and Tanmuran /θ/ and /x/ are primarily found in Sinitic loanwords.

New Kudara has the following consonant inventory:

Labial Alveolar Palatal Velar
Nasal m n (ŋ)
Stop p pʰ b t tʰ d k kʰ g
Fricative f v x ɣ
Sibilant s z ɕ ʑ
Affricate t͡s t͡sʰ (d͡z) t͡ɕ t͡ɕʰ (d͡ʑ)
Approximant j w
Liquid ɾ
Lateral l

The distinction between the tenuis and aspirated voiceless obstruents is only made word-initially (in the orthography, the tenuis obstruents are indicated by a preceding <f>). In other environments, the distinction is neutralised.

When a word-initial cluster is created as a result of the loss of */i/ and */u/, the following vowel is always lengthened. Additionally, some word-initial clusters undergo their own sound changes:

First Second Result
/f/ /p/, /b/ [p]
/f/ /t/, /d/ [t]
/f/ /k/, /g/ [k]
/v/ /b/ [b]
/v/ /d/ [d]
/v/ /g/ [g]
/m/, /n/ /p/, /b/ [b]
/m/, /n/ /f/, /v/ [v]
/m/, /n/ /t/, /d/ [d]
/m/, /n/ /s/, /t͡s/, /z/ [z]
/m/, /n/ /ɕ/, /t͡ɕ/, /ʑ/ [ʑ]
/m/ /ɾ/ [bɾ]
/n/ /ɾ/ [dɾ]
/m/ /l/ [bl]
/n/ /l/ [zl]
/m/, /n/ /k/, /g/ [g]
/m/, /n/ /x/, /ɣ/ [ɣ]
/s/ /b/ [sv]
/t͡s/ /b/ [t͡sv]
/ɕ/ /b/ [ɕv]
/t͡ɕ/ /b/ [t͡ɕv]
/m/ /j/ [n]
/p(ʰ)/ /j/ [s]
/b/, /n/ /j/ [z]
/f/ /j/ [x]
/v/ /j/ [ɣ]
/t/, /k/, /t͡s/, /t͡ɕ/ /j/ [t͡ɕ]
/tʰ/, /kʰ/, /t͡sʰ/, /t͡ɕʰ/ /j/ [t͡ɕʰ]
/d/, /z/, /ʑ/, /g/, /ɣ/ /j/ [ʑ]
/s/, /ɕ/, /x/ /j/ [ɕ]
/ɾ/ /j/ [l]
/l/ /j/ [j]
/k/ /w/ [f]
/g/ /w/ [v]

Voiced obstruents (with the exception of /v/ after an unvoiced sibilant) lose their voicing when in a cluster with an unvoiced obstruent. Additionally, all sibilant clusters assimilate into a single sibilant (the first sibilant determines whether the resulting consonant is a fricative or an affricate, while if either sibilant is palatalised, the resulting sibilant is also palatalised), and /j/ after any consonant unconditionally induces palatalisation. Any /w/ occurring after a labial is lost, while /w/ after /k/ and /g/ assimilate to form /f/ and /v/ respectively. Finally, /t͡s/ (and its voiced counterpart /z/) assimilate into any following stop, /f/, /v/, /x/ or /ɣ/, forming a geminate (this occurs after devoicing, so /z/ + /k/ becomes /sk/ instead of */gː/).

Among word-initial clusters, stops (unless followed by /ɾ/ or /l/), /ɾ/, /l/, /j/ and /w/ cannot occur as the first consonant. An epenthetic /a/ or /ə/ (dependent on vowel harmony) is inserted after stops, /ɾ/ and /l/, while the glides /j/ and /w/ are converted into their vocalic counterparts (/e/ and /i/ for /j/, /o/ and /u/ for /w/).

Phonotactics

Proto-Japonic has been hypothesised to have had a strict CV(j,w?)(N) syllable structure, where V could either be short or long. The only place V and VN syllables could occur was word-initially; all non-word-initial syllables must begin with a consonant. What is not clear is whether Proto-Japonic allowed word-final consonants.

Both Namari and Tanmuran have a slightly looser syllable structure, namely (C)(j)V(j,w)(N), where V can either be short or long. Namari also allows overlong (trimoraic) vowels. Note that this syllable structure in Namari only applies to the phonemic description of the language; phonetically, dialects (including the standard Yaezora dialect) may allow additional consonant clusters that do not fit into the nominal syllable structure.

In comparison, New Kudara has the syllable structure (C)(C)V(j,w)(C), where V can either be short or long. The more complex phonotactics in New Kudara is the consequence of the elision of */i/ and */u/. While words ending in stops are rare in native words, they are common in loanwords.

Numbers

Value Proto-Japonic Namari New Kudara Tanmuran
(indef.) *eku eku if eku
0 nashi nas
1 *pitə (*-te-) pito (-tetu) stō (stīc) ficho (tetsu)
2 *puta puta ftā futa
3 *mi mi mi
4 *yə yo jo yo
5 *itu etu səc ichu
6 *mu mu mu
7 *nana nana nana nana
8 *ya ya ja ya
9 *kəkənə kokono kokono kokono
10 *təwə
20 *pata pata pata hata
30 *mi-so miso nəsu misho
40 *yə-so yoso joso yoso
50 *i-so eso isho
60 *mu-so muso məsu muso
70 *nana-so nanaso nanaso nanaso
80 *ya-so yaso jaso yaso
90 *kəkənə-so kokonoso kokonoso kokonoso
100 *momo momo mumu momo
200 *puta-po putao ftāpo futō
300 *mi-po mio nəpu myō
400 *yə-po jopo
500 *i-po eo spū icho
600 *mu-po muo məpu
700 *nana-po nanao nanapo nanō
800 *ya-po yao japo yofō
900 *kəkənə-po kokonō kokonopo kokonō
1,000 *ti chi čə (-č) chi
2,000 *puta-ti putachi ftāč futachi
3,000 *mi-ti michi nəččə michi
4,000 *yə-ti yochi jočča yochi
5,000 *i-ti echi šœ ichi
6,000 *mu-ti muchi məččə muchi
7,000 *nana-ti nanachi nanač nanachi
8,000 *ya-ti yachi jačča yachi
9,000 *kəkənə-ti kokonochi kokonoč kokonochi
10,000 *yərən-tu yoyozu joroz yorozu

In New Kudara, numerals less than one hundred have their own forms (in comparison, Namari only has unique forms for eleven and twelve). They are listed below:

+1 +2 +3 +4 +5 +6 +7 +8 +9 +10
0 stō ftā jo səc nana ja kokono
10 tosto tofta tōz tojo tocca tōm tōna toja tokono pata
20 patasto patafta pataz patajo patacca patam patana pataja patakono nəsu
30 zustu zuftə zūz zuju zuccə zūm zunə zujə zukunu joso
40 josto josta josoz josojo josocca josom josona josoja josokono
50 sustu suftə sūz sūju succə sūm sunə sujə sukunu məsu
60 vustu vuftə vūz vūju vuccə vūm vunə vujə vukunu nanaso
70 nasosto nasofta nasoz nasojo nasocca nasom nasona nasoja nasokono jaso
80 šosto šofta šōz šojo šocca šōm šona šoja šokono kokonoso
90 kozosto kozofta kozoz kozojo kozocca kozom kozona kozoja kozokono mumu

Classifiers

The history of classifiers in the Japonic languages has been fairly obscured by Chinese influence, but I can reconstruct three classifiers that do not resemble the nouns they describe:

  • *-tu, the general classifier
  • *-(ta)ri, the classifier for people
  • *-uka, the classifier for days

From Old Japanese evidence, in particular from prose, it is apparent that classifiers were in limited use, typically used to refer to elevated objects and deities. In most cases, count forms of nouns are combined directly with numerals. Hence I believe that numerals in Proto-Japonic behaved the same way as adjectives: they were bound forms that directly attached to a noun.

The following table lists the forms of each numeral-classifier compound and their descendants:

Value *-tu *-(ta)ri *-uka
Proto-Japonic Namari New Kudara Tanmuran Proto-Japonic Namari New Kudara Tanmuran Proto-Japonic Namari New Kudara Tanmuran
(indef.) *eku-tu ekutu ifcə ekutsu *eku-tari ekutai iftəl ekutari *eku-uka ekuka ifkə ekuka
1 *pite-tu pitotu, pitetu stīc tetsu, fichetsu *pitə-ri pitoi stōl fichori *pitə-pi pitoe, pitopi stōs fichoi
2 *puta-tu putatu ftāc futatsu *puta-ri putai ftāl futari *puta-uka putuka ftōka futsuka
3 *mit-tu mittu nəc mitchu *mit-tari mittai nəttəl mitchari *mit-uka mikka nəkkə mikka
4 *yə-tu yottu joc yotsu *yə-tari yottai jotal yotari *yə-uka yokka joka yokka
5 *itu-tu etutu səcəc ichutsu *itu-tari etutai səttəl ichutari *itu-uka etuka səkkə ichuka
6 *mut-tu muttu məc muttsu *mut-tari muttai məttəl muttari *mut-uka → *muyuka muika mīkə muika
7 *nana-tu nanatu nanac nanatsu *nana-tari nanatai nanatal nanatari *nana-uka nanoka nanoka nanoka
8 *ya-tu yattu jac yatsu *ya-tari yattai jatal yatari *ya-uka yauka jaka yōka
9 *kəkənə-tu kokonotu kokonoc kokonotsu *kəkənə-tari kokonotai kokonotal kokonotari *kəkənə-uka kokonoka kokonoka kokonoka
10 *tə-wə *tə-tari totai total totari *tə-uka tōka tōka tōka
11 *tə-wə amar-i pite-tu tōtetu tostec tōtetsu *tə-tari amar-i pitə-ri tōtetai tostol tōtari-tori *tə-uka amar-i pitə-pi tōtoe tostos tōka-toi
12 *tə-wə amar-i puta-tu tōtatu toftac tōtatsu *tə-tari amar-i puta-ri tōtatai toftal tōtari-tari *tə-uka amar-i puta-uka tōtuka toftoka tōka-tsuka
13 *tə-wə amar-i mit-tu tō-mittu tōzac tō mitchu *tə-tari amar-i mit-tari totai-mittai tōzattal tōtari mitchari *tə-uka amar-i mit-uka tōka-mikka tōzakka tōka mikka
14 *tə-wə amar-i yə-tu tō-yottu tojoc tō yotsu *tə-tari amar-i yə-tari totai-yottai tojotal tōtari yotari *tə-uka amar-i yə-uka tōka-yokka tojoka tōka yokka
15 *tə-wə amar-i itu-tu tō-etutu toccac tō ichutsu *tə-tari amar-i itu-tari totai-etutai toccatal tōtari ichutari *tə-uka amar-i itu-uka tōka-etuka toccaka tōka ichuka
16 *tə-wə amar-i mut-tu tō-muttu tōmac tō muttsu *tə-tari amar-i mut-tari totai-muttai tōmattal tōtari muttari *tə-uka amar-i mut-uka tōka-muika tōmeka tōka muika
17 *tə-wə amar-i nana-tu tō-nanatu tōnac tō nanatsu *tə-tari amar-i nana-tari totai-nanatai tōnattal tōtari nanatari *tə-uka amar-i nana-uka tōka-nanoka tōnoka tōka nanoka
18 *tə-wə amar-i ya-tu tō-yattu tojac tō yatsu *tə-tari amar-i ya-tari totai-yattai tojatal tōtari yatari *tə-uka amar-i ya-uka tōka-yauka tojaka tōka yōka
19 *tə-wə amar-i kəkənə-tu tō-kokonotu tokonoc tō kokonotsu *tə-tari amar-i kəkənə-tari totai-kokonotai tokonotal tōtari kokonotari *tə-uka amar-i kəkənə-uka tōka-kokonoka tokonoka tōka kokonoka
20 *pata-ti patachi patač hatachi *pata-tari patatai patatal hatatari *pata-uka patuka patoka hatsuka
30 *mi-son-ti misoji nəsuž mishoji *mi-so-tari misotai nəsutəl mishotari *mi-so-uka misoka nəsukə mishoka
100 *momo momo mumu momo *momo-tari momotai mumutəl momotari *momo-uka momoka mumukə momoka
200 *puta-po putao ftāpo futō *puta-po-tari putaotai ftāpotal futōtari *puta-po-uka putaoka ftāpoka futōka
1,000 *ti chi čə (-č) chi *ti-tari chitai čāl chitari *ti-uka chuka čukə chuka
10,000 *yərən-tu yoyozu joroz yorozu *yərən-tu-tari yoyozutai jorozzal yorozutari *yərən-tu-uka yoyozuka joroska yorozuka

The word *pe ("a division", whence Old Japanese -pe1; derivatives include *pen-tatay- "to separate") was likely a standalone noun in Proto-Japonic (and is, in some cases, still used as such in Japanese), but in all three featured languages has evolved into an additional classifier. The various forms are listed below:

Value Proto-Japonic Namari New Kudara Tanmuran
(indef.) *eku-pe ekue ifpi ekue
1 *pitə-pe pitoe stōpe fichoe
2 *puta-pe putae ftāpe futae
3 *mi-pe mie nəbi mie
4 *yə-pe yoe jope yoe
5 *itu-pe etue səppi ichue
6 *mu-pe mue məbi mue
7 *nana-pe nanae nanape nanae
8 *ya-pe yae jape yae
9 *kəkənə-pe kokonoe kokonope kokonoe
10 *tə-pe toe tope toe
11 *tə-pe amar-i pitə-pe tōtoe tostope tōtoe
12 *tə-pe amar-i puta-pe tōtae toftape tōtae
13 *tə-pe amar-i mi-pe toe-mie tōzape toe mie
14 *tə-pe amar-i yə-pe toe-yoe tojope toe yoe
15 *tə-pe amar-i itu-pe toe-etue toccape toe ichue
16 *tə-pe amar-i mu-pe toe-mue tōmape toe mue
17 *tə-pe amar-i nana-pe toe-nanae tōnape toe nanae
18 *tə-pe amar-i ya-pe toe-yae tojape toe yae
19 *tə-pe amar-i kəkənə-pe toe-kokonoe tokonope toe kokonoe
20 *pata-pe patae patape hatae
30 *mi-so-pe misoe nəsupi mishoe
100 *momo-pe momoe mumupi momoe
200 *puta-po-pe putaowe ftāpope futōe
1,000 *ti-pe chie čpī chie
10,000 *yərən-tu-pe yoyozue jorospe yorozue

While numeral-classifier compounds are nouns in Namari and Tanmuran, they are verbal adjectives in New Kudara. Tanmuran and some Namari dialects allow numeral-classifier compounds to occur after the noun they modify.

Derivative forms

I reconstruct an ordinal suffix *-me? (which may be *-may instead; I have yet to determine which reconstruction is correct). This is reflected in Namari -me, New Kudara -me/-mi, and Tanmuran -me. All three languages also show a reflex of *patu ("first") which is the only ordinal I can reconstruct for Proto-Japonic (Namari: patu, New Kudara: pac, Tanmuran: hatsu).

Swadesh list

No English Proto-Japonic Namari New Kudara Tanmuran
1 I *wa-, *a wae ware ware
2 thou *na-? nae nare nare
3 he *ka- shie kare kare
4 we *wa-ta waeya watta waira
5 you *na-ta? naeya natta naira
6 they *ka-ta shieya katta kaira
7 this *kə-, *i-? ie, koe kore kore
8 that *ka-, *sə- soe ore, kare ore, kare
9 here *kə-kə iki, koko kof koko
10 there *sə-kə soko of, kaf asoko, kaka
11 who *ta- tae tare tare
12 what *na, *nə nani, nan nau, nan
13 where *en-tu ezuku iz ezu
14 when *e-tu etu ic etsu
15 how *e-ka eka ikə eka
16 not *-n-, *na- -n, nān -n, nēn -n, nain
17 all *so-?, *muina sōte, mina mul, nā moro, mina
18 many *əpə- ōkai oposam ōi
19 some sosshi sušam sokoi
20 few sunne sunnəsəm sonai
21 other *poka poka poka foka
22 one *pitə pito stō ficho
23 two *puta puta ftā futa
24 three *mi mi mi
25 four *yo yo jo yo
26 five *itu etu səc ichu
27 big *əpə- ōkokai opomasam ōkī
28 long *nanka- nagakai nagasam nagai
29 wide *pirə pirokai srōsam firoi
30 thick *atu- atukai accam atsui
31 heavy *əmə- omokai omosam omoi
32 small chīkakai, imi- mīsəm chishī
33 short mijikakai skasam ijakai
34 narrow *sem(p)a- semakai sibəsəm semai
35 thin *koma-ka- komaka kuməkəm komai
36 woman *wə-me-na(-nko) onna, otome onna f onna, onago
37 man (male) *wə- otoko wiku m okonogo
38 man (human) *pitə pito stō m ficho
39 child *warampe, *ko wayabe warabe f warabe
40 wife *tuma tuma cmā f tsuma
41 husband *wə-pitə otto, tuma osto m otto
42 mother *amo?, *pa-pa? amo ama f haha
43 father *si-si? kazo aso m shishi
44 animal *iki-mosi, *kay(-nta)-mənə kemono čœmuš n kedamono
45 fish *[u,o,i]wo uo uju n io
46 bird *təri toi tol f tori
47 dog *(w)enu eni in m enu
48 louse *sira-mi shiyami šrān n shirami
49 snake *pa(y)m-(p)[u,i] pebi pez n hamu
50 worm *mosi moshi muš n moshi
51 tree *kəy ki f ke
52 forest *məri moi mol f mori
53 stick (of wood) *yonta eda, tue judə m tsue, yoda
54 fruit *muy, *nar-i(-mənə) mī, naimono nalmon n, na f nari
55 seed *tanay tane tane m tane
56 leaf *pa pa pa f ha
57 root *nay ne n ne
58 bark kogā kubədə n kihada
59 flower *pana pana pana f hana
60 grass *kusa kusa fsā m kusa
61 rope *tuna, *napa tuna, nā cnā f, napa m tsuna, nawa
62 skin *kapa kapa n kawa
63 meat *sisi shishi šœ n shishi
64 blood *ti?, *tuy? chī čə m chi
65 bone *pəne? pone pone m fone
66 fat *ampura abuya avra f abura
67 egg *taman-ko tamago tamago f tamago
68 horn *tuno tuno cnō m tsuno
69 tail *wo? shû ušəl f shiru
70 feather *panay pane pane m hane
71 hair *kay (kamino) kē (karano) ke m (kamino) ke
72 head *tumpu(-ri) tubui, atama cvəl f tsubu
73 ear *mimi mimi nən f mimi
74 eye *may me n me
75 nose *pana pana pana n hana
76 mouth *kutuy kuchi fəč m kuchi
77 tooth *pa pa pa n ha
78 tongue *sita shita šta f shicha
79 fingernail *tumay tume cmē m tsume
80 foot *asi ashi m ashi
81 leg *asi ashi m ashi
82 knee *pinsa piza m pija
83 hand *tay te f te
84 wing *tumpasa tubasa cvasa n tsubasa
85 belly *para paya para n hara
86 guts *wata wata wata f wata
87 neck *kumpi kubi fəz n kubi
88 back *səy se m se
89 breast *munay mune f mune
90 heart *kəkərə kokoro kokoro f kokoro
91 liver *kimo kimo čmū m kimo
92 to drink *nəm- nomu nobam nomun
93 to eat *kam-, *kup- tabu, (kuwayu) fafam kamun
94 to bite *kam- kamu kabam kamun
95 to suck *sop- sou sufəm soun
96 to spit *pak- paku pafam hakun
97 to vomit *pak- paku pafam hakun
98 to blow *puk- puku fəfəm fukun
99 to breathe *pak- paku pafam hakun
100 to laugh *warap- wayau warafam waraun
101 to see *mi- nərəm miru
102 to hear *kik- kiku čəfəm kikun
103 to know *sir- shiyu šərəm shirun
104 to think *əməp- omou omofam omoun
105 to smell *kaN[k,p]- kagu kavam kabun
106 to fear *əsər-ay- osoyu osoreram osorerun
107 to sleep *na-i-mur- nemuyu nenram nemurun
108 to live *ik-[u,o]y- eku səčrəm ikiru
109 to die *sin-, *maromp- mayobu, shinu šədəm, marovam shinu
110 to kill kərəs- koyosu korosam korosun
111 to fight *tatak-ap- tatakau tatakafam tatakaun
112 to hunt *kar-? kayu karam karun
113 to hit *tatak- tataku tatafam tatakun
114 to cut *kir- kiyu čərəm kiru
115 to split *war- wayu waram warun
116 to stab *sas- sasu sasam sasun
117 to scratch *kak- kaku kafam kakun
118 to dig *pər- poyu poram porun
119 to swim *əyənk- oigu ojvam oigun
120 to fly *təmp- tobu tovam tobun
121 to walk *ar[u,i]k- aiku arfam arukun
122 to come *kə- fəm kun
123 to lie (down) *na-y- nei neram nerun
124 to sit *wo-y- suwayu, ī wirəm irun
125 to stand *tat- tatu tatam tatsun
126 to turn *mapar- māyu maparam mawarun
127 to fall *ətə-y- otu oteram oterun
128 to give *tampay-?, *kur(y?)ay-? atau taberam, freram taberun, kurerun
129 to hold *mət- motu motam motsun
130 to squeeze *simpər- shiboyu švoram shiborun
131 to rub *sur- -suru kosram, nasram -surun
132 to wash *arap- ayau arafam araun
133 to wipe *səsər-? sosoyu, sorru sosoram sosorun
134 to pull *pik- piku səfəm fikun
135 to push *əs- osu osam osun
136 to throw *nankay- nagu nageram nagerun
137 to tie *musomp- musobu zūvam musobun
138 to sew *nup-? nufəm nūn
139 to count *kansu-apay- kazou kazoperam kazoerun
140 to say *ip- iu ifəm iun
141 to sing *uta-p- utau ftāfam utaun
142 to play *as[u,o]mp- asobu azvam asobun
143 to float *[u,o]k- uku fəfəm ukun
144 to flow *nanka-ray- nagayu nagareram nagarerun
145 to freeze *kəpər- kōyu koporam kōrun
146 to swell *puk-o?-ra-(m,y) pukyamu, pukyu fradam, freram fukoramun, fukorerun
147 sun *pi smar f fi
148 moon *tuk[u,o]y tuki cəč m tsuki
149 star *pəsi poshi poš n foshi
150 water *men-tu mezu (mena) miz (minə) f mezu (mina)
151 rain *amay ame ame f ame
152 river *kapa kapa m kawa
153 lake medōmi mizūn f mezomi
154 sea *omi omi un f omi
155 salt *shipo shio špū n shio
156 stone *esoy eshi m eshi
157 sand *suna suna snā n suna
158 dust *tiri chī čəl f chiri
159 earth *tuti?, *[m,n]i-? tuchi zəč f tsuchi
160 cloud *kumo kumo fmū m kumo
161 fog *kuiri čəl f kiri
162 sky *sora soya surə n sora
163 wind *kansay kaze kaze n kaze
164 snow *yoki yoki juč f yoki
165 ice *kəpər-i kōi kopol f kōri
166 smoke *kaympu-[r,s]i kebui keval f kebushi
167 fire *poy m pi
168 ash *papi pai pas f pai
169 to burn *moya-y- moyu mujirəm moerun
170 road *mi-ti michi nəč m michi
171 mountain *yama yama yama m yama
172 red *aka aka aka(ero) n aka(ero)
173 green *awo ao, medoi aw(ero) n, mibə f ao(ero), medori
174 yellow *koy kī (kieyo) kīru n ki (kīro)
175 white *siro, sira- shiyo šrū(iru) (šrā-) n shiro
176 black *kuro kuyo frū(iru) n kuro
177 night *yo(-ru) yo(yu) ju(r) m yo(ru)
178 day *pi f fi
179 year *təsey toshi toš n toshi
180 warm *noku- nokukai nufsəm nokui
181 cold *sam-, *pi- samukai, pitakai samsam, stāsam samui, fitai
182 full *mitoy- mitu nəčrəm michirun
183 new *[m,n]ipi- nīkai, ayata zœsəm nī-, aratashī
184 old *puru- puyukai fərsəm furui
185 good *yə- ei, yokai ē, yotasam ei, yoi
186 bad *waru- wayukai warsam warui
187 rotten *kusar- kusayu fsāram kusarun
188 dirty *kitana- kitanakai čtanasam kitanai
189 straight *sunko sugo- skū- sugo-
190 round *maru- mayukai marsam marui
191 sharp suidokai sərdusam surudoi
192 dull *nempu- nebukai nivsəm nebui
193 smooth *nam(p)e- namekka nabekka nabekka
194 wet *nuray- nuyu dreram nurerun
195 dry *kawak- kauku kōfam kawakun
196 correct *tantasi- tadashi tadašam tadashī
197 near *tika- chikakai čkāsam chikai
198 far *təpə- tōkai toposam tōi
199 right *[m,n]inki-ri migī gīl f migi
200 left *pin-tari pidai stāl f fijari
201 at *ni (locative) (locative) (instrumental)
202 in *ni (locative) (locative) (instrumental)
203 with *tə, *si (comitative), (instrumental) (comitative), (instrumental) (comitative), (instrumental)
204 and *tə -to -to/-tu to
205 if *-mpa -ba -ba/-bə -ba
206 because -kka -kar -kara
207 name *na na(mae) na(mē) n na(mae)

Family

English Proto-Japonic Namari New Kudara Tanmuran
family yauchi jōč n uchinoya
parent *əyə-a oya oja m oya
mother *amo?, *pa-pa? amo ama f haha
father *si-si? kazo aso m shishi
child *warampe, *ko wayabe warabe f warabe
son oyobe wigəku m otoko
daughter maibe migəku f menoko
son-in-law *moko moko muku m moko
daughter-in-law *yəme yome jome f yome
older sibling *sosa sosa susə n sosa
older brother okkonoe wisu m enoko
older sister onnae misu f enome
younger sibling *ətə oto oto n oto
younger brother otouto wittu m oshimoko
younger sister imouto mittu f meshimome
cousin *itoko itoko stūku f ichoko
grandparent oyamae jamē n sakinoya
grandfather shimae šəmī m shinoya
grandmother pamae pamē f hanoya
grandchild *(m)uman-ko mmago māgo n mago
grandson umango wemago m magonoko
granddaughter umamme memago f magonome
uncle *wən-si oji m oji
aunt *wəm-pa oba oba f oba
nephew *wə-pi oiko osko m oiko
niece *me-pi meiko misku f meiko

Time

English Proto-Japonic Namari New Kudara Tanmuran
time *təki toki toč f toki
day *pi f fi
night *yo-ru yo(yu) ju(r) m yo(ru)
dawn *aka-(təki) akatoki ake f akegata
morning *asa asa asa f asa
afternoon *pi-ru piyu sər f firu
dusk *kura-(təki) kuyatoki frē f kuregata
evening *yupu if m
month *tuk[u,o]y tuki cəč m tsuki
season *kərə koyo koro f koro
spring *paru payu par f haru
summer *natu natu nac f natsu
autumn *ak-i aki f aki
winter *puyu pui f fui
year *təsey toshi toš n toshi
three days ago *saki wətə tu pi sakiottoi sačotoc f sakiototsui
day before yesterday *wətə tu pi ottoi otoc f ototsui
yesterday *ki nə pu kinou čənuf f kinō
today *ke pu keu kif f kyō
tomorrow *as- asu as f asu
day after tomorrow *asa-te? asatte asate f asatte
two days after tomorrow *si-asa-te? shiasatte šāsate f shiasatte
previous morning sakesa sačasa f sakesa
this morning *ke-asa kesa kisə f kesa
next morning *asa-ita ashita ašta f ashita
two years ago *wətə-təsey ottoshi ototoš n ototoshi
last year *kənsə kozo kozo n kozo
this year *kə-təsey kotoshi kotoš n kotoshi

In general, the following prefixes are used:

-3 -2 -1 0 +1 +2 +3
Namari sakiot(o)- ot(o)- saki- ko- akut- tugakut- shitugakut-
New Kudara sačoto-/səčutu- oto-/utu- sač-/səč- ko-/ku- aka-/əkə- skāka-/skœkə- šaskaka-/šəskəkə-
Tanmuran sakioto- oto- saki- ko- kuru- tō- shitō-

Months

So far, I can only reconstruct the names of two months for Proto-Japonic:

  • *simə-tuk[u,o]y, for the eleventh month
  • *si-pasu, for the twelfth month

For the Innocence Seekers setting, I've used the Japanese names and extended them to the other Japonic languages. They are listed below:

No Proto-Japonic Namari New Kudara Tanmuran
1 *mutu-tuk[u,o]y mutuki zœč mutsuki
2 *kisaranki? kisayagi čāraž kisharagi
3 *i-ya-əp-i yayoi šās yāwi
4 *un-tuk[u,o]y? uzuki fcœč uzuki
5 *sa-tuk[u,o]y satuki sačča satsuki
6 *mi-nan-tuk[u,o]y menazuki nāžža minazuki
7 *pun-tuk[u,o]y puzuki, pumizuki fəzəč fuzuki
8 *pan-tuk[u,o]y pazuki pazač hazuki
9 *nanka-tuk[u,o]y nagatuki nagačča nagatsuki
10 *kamu-nan-tuk[u,o]y kannazuki kannažža kannazuki
11 *simə-tuk[u,o]y shimotuki šmōčča shimotsuki
12 *si-pasu shiasu špās shiwasu

Note that all New Kudara forms are feminine ("yin"), even though "moon" itself is masculine ("yang").

Days of the week

Obviously, these cannot be reconstructed for Proto-Japonic, as the ancient Japonic speakers never had a concept of "week". However, the three languages featured do have the days of the week derived from words with similar meaning.

English Namari New Kudara Tanmuran
Sunday pinoi osnas ofikoro
Monday tukonoi cəfnus tsukunofi
Tuesday ponoi spōšnos fiboshi(nofi)
Wednesday menai nāpošnos minafoshi(nofi)
Thursday konoi kibušnus keboshi(nofi)
Friday kananoi kanebošnos kanefoshi(nofi)
Saturday tuchinoi zəčpušnus tsuchiboshi(nofi)

Geography

English Proto-Japonic Namari New Kudara Tanmuran
river *kapa kapa m kawa
lake medōmi mizūn f mezomi
sea *omi omi un f omi
island *sima shima šmā m shima
mountain *yama yama jama m yama
forest *məri moi mol f mori
valley *tani tani taz n tani
meadow *patakay patake patake n hatake
field *para paya para f hara
cape *(mi-)saki misaki, saki sač f, zāč f saki, mishaki

Cardinal directions

The cardinal directions are in general unreconstructible for Proto-Japonic. The only words I can reconstruct with confidence are *nisi and *paye. Reflecting this, the three languages featured have some divergent forms.

English Proto-Japonic Namari New Kudara Tanmuran
north *kami? kami kangata f kicha
east *a-? pingai pegal f azuma
south *paye?, *simo? shimo paj f hae
west *nisi? nishi žœ f nishi

Place names

English Proto-Japonic Namari New Kudara Tanmuran
Namari (*namari) Namai(kuni) Namal(faz) f Namari(kuni)
Japan *yama-tə Yamato(kuni) Jamato(faz) m Yamato(kuni)
Okinawa (*əki-napa) Okinā Očnapa m Okinawa

Conjugation

Being Insular Japonic languages, Namari and Tanmuran share a great deal of conjugational forms, while New Kudara, being Peninsular Japonic, is somewhat divergent.

The following compares conjugated forms of *ar- (inanimate existential) between languages:

Form Proto-Japonic Namari New Kudara Tanmuran
Infinitive *ar-i ai al ari
Conclusive *ar- ai am an
Attributive *ar-ro ayo aro aro
Imperative *ar-ryə ae ajo are
Negative *ar-an- *ayan aran *aran
Prohibitive *ar-ru-na anna arna anna
Volitional *ar-ram-ru ayamu aram ara
Emphatic *ar-ru-ay ae(me) are are
Participle *ar-i-tay atte ačče atche

The copula is derived from *ar- (Namari ya from *i ar-, New Kudara tom from *tə ar-, Tanmuran nyan from *ni ar-). The negative of *ar- is only used in copular forms (Namari yān from *i pa ar-an- via earlier yayan, New Kudara taran from *tə pa ar-an-, Tanmuran nyaran from *ni pa ar-an-).

The following compares conjugated forms of *wor- (animate existential) between languages:

Form Proto-Japonic Namari New Kudara Tanmuran
Infinitive *wor-i oi ul ori
Conclusive *wor- oi um on
Attributive *wor-ro oyo uru oro
Imperative *wor-ryə oe uju ore
Negative *wor-an- oyan urən oran
Prohibitive *wor-ru-na onna urnə onna
Volitional *wor-ram-ru oyamu urəm ora
Emphatic *wor-ru-ay oe(me) uri ore
Participle *wor-i-tay otte učči otche

The following compares conjugated forms of *kə- ("to come") between languages:

Form Proto-Japonic Namari New Kudara Tanmuran
Infinitive *k-i ki čə (č-) ki
Conclusive *k-ru fəm kun
Attributive *k-ro kuyo fru kuro
Imperative *kə(-ryə) ko(yo) ko ko
Negative *kə-n- kon kon kon
Prohibitive *k-ru-na kunna fərnə kunna
Volitional *kə-ram-ru koyamu koram kora
Emphatic *k-ru-ay kue(me) fre kure
Participle *k-i-tay kite čē kiche

The following compares conjugated forms of *as- ("to do") between languages:

Form Proto-Japonic Namari New Kudara Tanmuran
Infinitive *as-i shi shi
Conclusive *as-ru səm sun
Attributive *as-ro suyo səru suro
Imperative *as-ryə se(yo) šo se
Negative *as-an- sen san san
Prohibitive *as-ru-na sunna sarna sunna
Volitional *as-ram-ru seyamu saram sara
Emphatic *as-ru-ay sue(me) sare sure
Participle *as-i-tay shite ašše aiche

The following compares conjugated forms of *-n- (negative suffix) between languages:

Form Proto-Japonic Namari New Kudara Tanmuran
Infinitive *-n-i -n -z -n
Conclusive *-n- -n -n -n
Attributive *-n-ro -n -du/-do -do
Volitional *-n-ram-ru -ndaman -daman/-dəmən -daman
Emphatic *-n-ru-ay -ne(me) -de/-di -de
Participle *-n-i-tay -nde -žže/-žži -nje

The regular conjugational classes, beginning with forms of *ankay- ("to rise"), forming the lower vowel-stem class:

Form Proto-Japonic Namari New Kudara Tanmuran
Infinitive *ankay-i age age age
Conclusive *ankai-ru agu ageram agerun
Attributive *ankai-ro aguyo agero agero
Imperative *ankai-ryə ageyo agejo ageyo
Negative *ankai-n- agen agen agen
Prohibitive *ankai-ru-na agenna agerna agenna
Volitional *ankai-ram-ru ageyamu ageram agera
Emphatic *ankay-ru-ay ague(me) agere agere
Participle *ankay-i-tay agete agete agete

Forms of *sunkoy- ("to exceed, to surpass"), forming the upper vowel-stem class:

Form Proto-Japonic Namari New Kudara Tanmuran
Infinitive *sunkoy-i sugi səž sugi
Conclusive *sunkoi-ru sugu səžrəm sugirun
Attributive *sunkoi-ro suguyo səžru sugiro
Imperative *sunkoi-ryə sugiyo səžu sugiyo
Negative *sunkoi-n- sugin səžən sugin
Prohibitive *sunkoi-ru-na suginna səžərnə suginna
Volitional *sunkoi-ram-ru sugiyamu səžrəm sugira
Emphatic *sunkoi-ru-ay sugue(me) səžri sugire
Participle *sunkoy-i-tay sugite səžži sugiche

Forms of *kak- ("to scratch, to write"), forming the k-stem class:

Form Proto-Japonic Namari New Kudara Tanmuran
Infinitive *kak-i kaki kač kaki
Conclusive *kak-ru kaku kafam kakun
Attributive *kak-ro kako kafro kako
Imperative *kak-ryə kake kačo kake
Negative *kak-an- kakan kakan kakan
Prohibitive *kak-ru-na kakona kafarna kakuna
Volitional *kak-ram-ru kakamu kafram kaka
Emphatic *kak-ru-ay kake(me) kafre kake
Participle *kak-i-tay kaite kačče kaiche

Forms of *əyənk- ("to swim"), forming the g-stem class:

Form Proto-Japonic Namari New Kudara Tanmuran
Infinitive *əyənk-i oigi ojž oigi
Conclusive *əyənk-ru oigu ojvam oigun
Attributive *əyənk-ro oigo ojvro oigo
Imperative *əyənk-ryə oige ojžo oige
Negative *əyənk-an- oigan ojgan oigan
Prohibitive *əyənk-ru-na oigona ojvarna oiguna
Volitional *əyənk-ram-ru oigamu ojvram oiga
Emphatic *əyənk-ru-ay oige(me) ojvri oige
Participle *əyənk-i-tay oiede ojžže oije

Forms of *pəs- ("to dry"), forming the s-stem class:

Form Proto-Japonic Namari New Kudara Tanmuran
Infinitive *pəs-i poshi poš foshi
Conclusive *pəs-ru posu posam fosun
Attributive *pəs-ro poso posro foso
Imperative *pəs-ryə pose pošo fose
Negative *pəs-an- posan posan fosan
Prohibitive *pəs-ru-na posona posarna fosuna
Volitional *pəs-ram-ru posamu posram fosa
Emphatic *pəs-ru-ay pose(me) posre fose
Participle *pəs-i-tay poshite počče foiche

Forms of *mat- ("to wait"), forming the t-stem class:

Form Proto-Japonic Namari New Kudara Tanmuran
Infinitive *mat-i machi mač machi
Conclusive *mat-ru matu matam matsun
Attributive *mat-ro mato matro mato
Imperative *mat-ryə mate mačo mate
Negative *mat-an- matan matan matan
Prohibitive *mat-ru-na matona matarna matuna
Volitional *mat-ram-ru matamu matram mata
Emphatic *mat-ru-ay mate(me) matre mate
Participle *mat-i-tay matte mačče matche

Forms of *sin- ("to die"), forming the n-stem class:

Form Proto-Japonic Namari New Kudara Tanmuran
Infinitive *sin-i shini šəz shini
Conclusive *sin-ru shinu šədəm shinun
Attributive *sin-ro shino šədru shino
Imperative *sin-ryə shine šəzu shine
Negative *sin-an- shinan šənən shinan
Prohibitive *sin-ru-na shinona šədərnə shinuna
Volitional *sin-ram-ru shinamu šədrəm shina
Emphatic *sin-ru-ay shine(me) šədri shine
Participle *sin-i-tay shinde šəžži shinje

Forms of *warap- ("to laugh"), forming the p-stem class:

Form Proto-Japonic Namari New Kudara Tanmuran
Infinitive *warap-i wayai waras warai
Conclusive *warap-ru wayau warafam waraun
Attributive *warap-ro wayao warafro warao
Imperative *warap-ryə wayae waraso warae
Negative *warap-an- wayān warapan warawan
Prohibitive *warap-ru-na wayaona warafarna warauna
Volitional *warap-ram-ru wayāmu warafram warawa
Emphatic *warap-ru-ay wayae(me) warafre warae
Participle *warap-i-tay wayaute waračče waratche

Forms of *təmp- ("to fly"), forming the b-stem class:

Form Proto-Japonic Namari New Kudara Tanmuran
Infinitive *təmp-i tobi toz tobi
Conclusive *təmp-ru tobu tovam tobun
Attributive *təmp-ro tobo tovro tobo
Imperative *təmp-ryə tobe tozo tobe
Negative *təmp-an- toban toban toban
Prohibitive *təmp-ru-na tobona tovarna tobuna
Volitional *təmp-ram-ru tobamu tovram toba
Emphatic *təmpi-ru-ay tobe(me) tovre tobe
Participle *təmp-i-tay tonde tožže tonje

Forms of *yəm- ("to read"), forming the m-stem class:

Form Proto-Japonic Namari New Kudara Tanmuran
Infinitive *yəm-i yomi jon yomi
Conclusive *yəm-ru yomu jobam yomun
Attributive *yəm-ro yomo jobro yomo
Imperative *yəm-ryə yome jono yome
Negative *yəm-an- yoman joman yoman
Prohibitive *yəm-ru-na yomona jobarna yomuna
Volitional *yəm-ram-ru yomamu jobram yoma
Emphatic *yəm-ru-ay yome(me) jobre yome
Participle *yəm-i-tay yonde jožže yonje

Forms of *kir- ("to cut"), forming the r-stem class:

Form Proto-Japonic Namari New Kudara Tanmuran
Infinitive *kir-i čəl kiri
Conclusive *kir-ru kiyu čərəm kirun
Attributive *kir-ro kiyo čəru kiro
Imperative *kir-ryə kie čəlu kire
Negative *kir-an- kiyan čərən kiran
Prohibitive *kir-ru-na kinna čərnə kinna
Volitional *kir-ram-ru kiyamu čərəm kira
Emphatic *kir-ru-ay kie(me) čəri kire
Participle *kir-i-tay kitte čəčči kitche

Principal parts and base stems

Like most inflected languages, verbs in the three languages featured can be described using a number of principal parts. This is not to be confused with the concept of a "base stem", whose form can in some cases be determined entirely by the forms of other base stems.

Namari has the following base stems, using kaku and agu as examples:

Stem kaku agu Principal part?
Imperfective 1 kaka age No
Imperfective 2 kaka ageya No
Infinitive kaki age No
Conclusive kaku agu Yes
Attributive kako aguyo No
Perfective kake ague No
Imperative kake ageyo Yes
Participle kaite agete No

New Kudara has the following base stems, using kafam and ageram as examples:

Stem kafam ageram Principal part?
Conclusive kafa- agera- Yes (-m)
Finite kaf- age- No
Infinitive kač- age- Yes
Negative kak- age- Yes (-(a/ə)n)
Participle kačč- aget- Yes (-e/-i)

Tanmuran has the following base stems, using kakun and agerun as examples:

Stem kakun agerun Principal part?
Imperfective kaka age No
Infinitive kaki age No
Conclusive kak- ager- Yes (-un)
Imperative kake ageyo Yes
Participle kaiche agete No

Tense and aspect

For Proto-Japonic, I can confidently reconstruct five aspects, the aorist, the stative, the progressive, the preparative and the experiential, as well as a possible perfect form. In all three languages featured, this perfect evolved into a simple past, and the aspects are retained. All of these forms are based on the participle *-tay.

The reflexes of each different form in the daughter languages are as follows:

Proto-Japonic Namari New Kudara Tanmuran
Stative *-tay-ar- -tei -tem/-tim -tēn
Progressive *-tai-wor- -toi -tom/-tum -tōn
Preparative *-tay-ək- -toku -tofam/-tufəm -tokun
Experientive *-tai-mi- -temī -tenram/-tinrəm -temirun
Perfect *-tar- -ta -tam/-təm -tan

In Namari, the reflex of the stative and its progressive force ergative-absolutive alignment for nonpronominal subjects. Similarly, the New Kudara reflex uses a nominative/intransitive direct object in conjunction with an ergative subject. This behaviour is identical to the effect of the potential in these two languages. The Tanmuran reflex of the stative has passive semantics, and thus does not take a direct object.

All three languages featured also have a perfective-imperfective distinction, derived from the exact same construction (*-i wor-). Conventionally, the unmarked (aorist) aspect is the perfective, with the imperfective being marked. The forms are listed below:

  • Namari: -yoi (derived from polite stem; conjugates identically to oi)
  • New Kudara: -om/-um (attached to infinitive; conjugates identically to um)
  • Tanmuran: -yon (attached to stem; conjugates identically to on)

I cannot reconstruct a past tense for Proto-Japonic. Classical Japanese had a preterite -ki (attributive -si) separate from the perfect -tari, but it appears that it is unique to Japanese; I cannot find any trace of this preterite in the Ryukyuan languages. Regardless, the languages featured do have tense. In addition to the past, Namari and New Kudara have a marked future, with the same etymology. The tenses and their Proto-Japonic origins are listed below:

Proto-Japonic Namari New Kudara Tanmuran
Past *-i-tar- -ita -(t)tam/-(t)təm -icha/-ita
Future *-ram-ro-i woy- -(y)ami -ranram/-rənrəm
Posterior Future *-ram-ro-i woy-i-tar- -(y)amitta -rančam/-rənčəm

Mood

In general, mood in Japonic can be represented using three different strategies:

  • Basic inflection
  • Word-final suffix or particle
  • Auxillary verb

Only the imperative, prohibitive and volitional use basic inflection, which are listed in the forms given above. The other moods use either word-final suffixes or particles, or auxillary verbs.

Word-final suffixes and particles

It is important to note that word-final suffixes do not exclusively denote mood (in all three languages, the gerund is formed by a word-final suffix, for example). Hence this section will only list the modal suffixes.

I can reconstruct an indicative suffix *-m(u) for Proto-Ryukyuan, but not Proto-Japonic (if it did exist in Proto-Japonic, then Japanese must have lost it). I cannot reconstruct any other word-final suffixes or particles representing mood.

The three languages use the following suffixes to mark mood:

Namari New Kudara Tanmuran
Indicative - -m -n
Interrogative -ka -ro/-ru/-ga/-gə -mi/-ka
Inferential -sō -so/-su -sau
Epistemic -sa -sa/-sə -sa

While Namari has lost the polar/informative interrogative distinction, the other two languages featured still distinguish them. New Kudara uses the pair -ro/-ru for polar questions, and -ga/-gə for information questions. Similarly, Tanmuran uses -mi and -ka respectively in their roles.

In both New Kudara and Tanmuran, the unmarked mood is the subjunctive (however, the lemma uses the indicative), while in Namari it is the indicative (the inflected subjunctive is in very limited use and represented by an auxillary, although the unmarked subjunctive still affects agreement). Included are the indirect evidential suffixes; the inferential represents the realis indirect evidential (although semantically all indirect evidentials are irrealis, the inferential specifically indicates that the speaker has determined from indirect evidence that his/her utterance is true), while the epistemic (which would be called "potential", but this label is already used) is used for irrealis indirect evidentials.

The Namari and Tanmuran inferential suffix is actually a nominalising suffix; it must be suffixed by a form of the copula if used predicatively.

In all three languages, invocation of kakari-musubi (via the use of a focus marker) eliminates the word-final mood suffix outside of the interrogative. The existence of a declarative or exclamative focus marker (but not an interrogative focus marker) indicates that the verb is in the indicative.

Modal verbs

In addition to word-final suffixes and particles, modal verbs may be used to denote mood.

The desiderative mood I reconstruct as using the auxillary *pəsi- ("to want"), which is actually an adjective. It attaches to the infinitive.

Modal forms in all three languages are listed below:

Proto-Japonic Namari New Kudara Tanmuran
Desiderative *-in pəsi- -iboshi -bošam/-bušəm -iboshī
Optative *-ra-mpa yə- -(y)abakai -rabesam/-rəbisəm -(r)abai

Unique to Namari is the negative volitional auxillary -makai, which attaches to the attributive. The other two languages use the standard negated form of the volitional form of the negative as their negative volitional (a similar form also exists in Namari).

All three languages feature a periphrastic form of the desiderative, derived from *-i-tay pəsi- (Namari -te poshi, New Kudara -te pošam/-ti pušəm, Tanmuran -te foshī). In this construction, the sentence "X wants Y to [do] Z", where "[do]" is any transitive verb, argument X is in the genitive in Namari, ergative in New Kudara and nominative in Tanmuran, while Y is in the nominative in Namari, intransitive/nominative in New Kudara and nominative in Tanmuran. Z remains in the accusative in all three languages.

Voice

I reconstruct a causative *-(s)as- and a passive *-(r)aryay-, both of which attach directly to the stem. There reflexes are as follows:

Proto-Japonic Namari New Kudara Tanmuran
Causative 1 *-(s)as-(ay)- -(s)asu -(s)asam/-(s)əsəm -(s)asun
Causative 2 *-simay- šmeram shimerun
Passive *-(r)aryay- -(y)ayu -(r)areram/-(r)ərirəm -(r)arerun
Potential *-i wo ay- -yōu -uwirəm/-oweram -(i)werun

The potential can be considered a voice in Namari, due to its split-ergative nature, and also possesses passive properties in other Japonic languages. However, the three languages featured do feature a potential descended from the same Proto-Japonic form. In all three languages, the potential of "to do" is suppletive (Namari deku, New Kudara dečram, Tanmuran dekerun).

Even though New Kudara is tripartite, there is no antipassive. Instead, if the ergative argument is omitted, then marking the patient using the accusative case is optional.

Agreement

In general, the Japonic languages lack agreement between the verb and its arguments. However, many Japonic languages do have optional "directionality" agreement, using various forms of "to give" attached to the participle as auxillaries.

Directionality in Japonic is based on the person of the subject and the person of the beneficiary (not necessarily the direct object). If the person of the beneficiary is lower than that of the subject (e.g. first person beneficiary and third person subject), then the marked form is used. The "passive" forms listed below are used when the subject is omitted; this form is typically used for first person beneficiaries. The forms used are listed below:

Proto-Japonic Namari New Kudara Tanmuran
Unmarked *-tay tampay- -te agu, -te tabu -te taberam, -ti təbirəm -te taberun
Marked *-tay kur(y?)ay- -te kuyu -te freram, -ti frirəm -te kurerun
Passive *-tay morap- -te moyau -te morafam, -ti murəfəm -te moraun

The marked forms are the basis of polite requests:

  • Namari: -te kueyo, from *-tay kuray-ryə; -te kuyäbē, from *-tay kuray pampi-ar-ryə; -te kudasae, borrowed from Japanese
  • New Kudara: -te frejo/-ti friju, from *-tay kuray-ryə; -te frebelo/-ti fribilu, from *-tay kuray pampi-ar-ryə
  • Tanmuran: -te kure, from *-tay kuray-ryə; -te kurebere, from *-tay kuray pampi-ar-ryə; -te kudasai, borrowed from Japanese

Note: the form *kuryay- is my conjecture on the etymology of Okinawan kwiyun, which would have had a preform *kuye-.

Only Namari shows proper subject-verb agreement. In this case, most of the suffixes are borrowed from non-Japonic languages, and as such I will not mention them here.

Verbal adjectives

Unlike normal verbs, verbal adjectives do not conjugate for aspect or voice. They also have additional forms unique to adjectives:

Proto-Japonic Namari New Kudara Tanmuran
Adverb *-ku -ku -f(a/ə) -ku
Participle *-ku-tay -kute -(a)fte/-(ə)fti -kute
Degree (meas.) *-sa -sa/-sha -sa/-sə/-ša/-šə -sa/-sha
Degree (immeas.) *-mi -mi -(a/ə)n -mi

Only Namari and New Kudara possess comparative forms for adjectives; Tanmuran generally leaves comparatives unmarked. This form is derived from *-ku mə ar-, giving Namari -kumoi and New Kudara -(a)fmoram or -(ə)fmurəm. All three languages have superlative forms for adjectives, but they are derived differently:

  • Namari: from *-ku mə taka-, whence -kumotakakai
  • New Kudara: from *-ku mage-, whence -(a)fmagesam or -(ə)fməgisəm.
  • Tanmuran: from *-ku-tay əpə-, whence -kutōi

Negatives are formed differently from normal verbs, and as usual, they conjugate for "tense":

Proto-Japonic Namari New Kudara Tanmuran
Present *-ku pa na-n- -pan -fanēn/-fənīn -kanain
Perfect/Past *-ku pa na-n-i-tar- -panda -fanēžžam/-fənīžžəm -kanainjan

New Kudara, unlike the other Japonic languages in Innocence Seekers (except certain dialects of Namari), has gender, and this is reflected in the form of verbal (and nominal) adjectives used. Verbal adjectives (but not verbs) have different forms in the conclusive and attributive depending on gender. The "yin" gender, or feminine, is the default in New Kudara, and is used for the lemma.

New Kudara Proto-Japonic Namari cognates
Conclusive Attributive Emphatic Conclusive Attributive Emphatic Conclusive Attributive Emphatic Notes
Present
Feminine ("yin") -sam/-səm -saro/-səru -sare/-səri *-sa ar- *-sa ar-ro *-sa ar-ru-ay Unattested in Namari and Tanmuran.
Masculine ("yang") -(a/ə)š -ke/-ki -kere/-kiri *-si *-ke *-ke-ru-ay -shi -ke -kē This is also the base of Tanmuran -i and -ke.
Neuter -fam/-fəm -faro/-fəru -fare/-fəri *-ku ar- *-ku ar-ro *-ku ar-ru-ay -kai -kayo -kae The Namari attributive form is rare.
Past
Feminine ("yin") -saččam/-səččəm -saččaro/-səččəru -saččare/-səččəri *-sa ar-i-tar- *-sa ar-i-tar-ro *-sa ar-i-tar-ru-ay Unattested in Namari and Tanmuran.
Masculine ("yang") -(a)ftam/-(ə)ftəm -(a)ftaro/-(ə)ftəru -(a)ftare/-(ə)ftəri *-ku-tar- *-ku-tar-ro *-ku-tar-ru-ay Unattested in Namari and Tanmuran.
Neuter -faččam/-fəččəm -faččaro/-fəččəru -faččare/-fəččəri *-ku ar-i-tar- *-ku ar-i-tar-ro *-ku ar-i-tar-ru-ay -katta -kattayo -kattae
Future
Feminine ("yin") -saranram/-sərənrəm -saranro/-sərənru -saranre/-sərənri *-sa ar-ram-i-woy-ru *-sa ar-ram-i-woy-ro *-sa ar-ram-i-woy-ru-ay
Masculine ("yang") -kenram/-kinrəm -kenro/-kinru -kenre/-kinri *-ke-ram-i-woy-ru *-ke-ram-i-woy-ro *-ke-ram-i-woy-ru-ay -kemi -kemiyo -kemie
Neuter -faranram/-fərənrəm -faranro/-fərənru -faranre/-fərənri *-ku ar-ram-i-woy-ru *-ku ar-ram-i-woy-ro *-ku ar-ram-i-woy-ru-ay
Posterior Future
Feminine ("yin") -sarančam/-sərənčəm -sarančaro/-sərənčəru -sarančare/-sərənčəri *-sa ar-ram-i-woy-i-tar- *-sa ar-ram-i-woy-i-tar-ro *-sa ar-ram-i-woy-i-tar-ru-ay
Masculine ("yang") -kenčam/-kinčəm -kenčaro/-kinčəru -kenčare/-kinčəri *-ke-ram-i-woy-i-tar- *-ke-ram-i-woy-i-tar-ro *-ke-ram-i-woy-i-tar-ru-ay -kemitta -kemittayo -kemittae
Neuter -farančam/-fərənčəm -farančaro/-fərənčəru -farančare/-fərənčəri *-ku ar-ram-i-woy-i-tar- *-ku ar-ram-i-woy-i-tar-ro *-ku ar-ram-i-woy-i-tar-ru-ay

Note that negative and comparative forms of verbal adjectives do not have gendered forms.

Nominal morphology

Number

All languages featured have singular and plural number, while Namari also has a dual. Both Namari and Tanmuran also feature a collective separate from the plural, while New Kudara combines the two.

Proto-Japonic Namari New Kudara Tanmuran
Dual -na -(t)ta/-(t)tə -ra
Plural *-ta -ya
Collective *-ta-ti -tachi -tachi

In all cases, number is not marked if the noun is explicitly modified by a numeral. The New Kudara plural suffix is -tta/-ttə after a noun ending in a vowel, and -ta/-tə after nouns ending in a non-coronal or non-obstruent consonant. However, if the word-final consonant is a coronal obstruent (/t/, /d/, /s/, /z/, /ɕ/, /ʑ/, /t͡s/, /t͡ɕ/), the /t/ assimilates to this consonant, forming a geminate.

Case

The three languages featured have a variable number of cases. Namari has thirteen cases, New Kudara eleven, and Tanmuran ten. However, they vary slightly.

The following table lists the cases and their possible Proto-Japonic markers:

Proto-Japonic Namari New Kudara Tanmuran
Ergative *i? -i -j/-i/-e (Null)
Intransitive *(Null) (Null)
Accusative *wo -o -o/-u -o
Vocative *yə? -yo/(Null)
Genitive *nə/*na/*nka -no/-na/-ga -no/-nu/-ga/-gə/-na/-nə -no/-ga/-na
Dative *ni -n(i) -(a/ə)z -n(i)
Instrumental *si -ji -(a/ə)š -se
Comitative *tə -to -to/-tu -to
Ablative 1 *yo-ri -yoi -(j)ol/-(j)ul -yori
Ablative 2 *kara -kaya -kara
Allative *pe -(m)pe -e/-i -e
Terminative *man-te? -made -made/-mədi -made
Locative -(n)de -(a/ə)d
Abessive -(n)ku

Note that the "ergative" is simply a label corresponding to its role in New Kudara; I am actually unsure of the morphosyntactic alignment of Proto-Japonic (New Kudara is tripartite). Not listed above is the pronominal nominative suffix *-re?, which is realised as -e in Namari, -re/-ri in New Kudara and -re in Tanmuran.

In all three languages, which genitive to use is determined by the animacy of the noun. Typically, animates use the reflex of *nka, while inanimates use the reflex of *nə. The marker *na is vestigial in all three languages.

Namari has three declensional classes, and is unique in that its nouns can alternate between /i/-suffixed and bound forms in declension (this is the basis of the Namari second declension). Namari has a few irregular nouns, which retain things such as irregular genitives or diphthongal pronunciation in the dative.

New Kudara does not have declensional classes per se, but there are rules that need to be followed when declining for case:

  • The lemma is the intransitive case.
  • The ergative case uses -e or -i when the noun stem ends in a consonant.
  • Likewise, the accusative case uses -o or -u in the same environment.
  • If the noun stem ends in a vowel, the ablative suffix begins with -j-; otherwise it is omitted.
  • If the noun ends in a consonant, the dative, instrumental and locative have an epenthetic /a/ or /ə/ added.
  • If the noun ends in -a or , they are replaced by long variants of the ergative and accusative suffixes.
  • If the noun ends in -e or -i, they become nonsyllabic -j- before the accusative -o or -u.
  • If the noun ends in -o or -u, the ergative is simply -j.
  • If the noun ends in a long vowel, hiatus is always inserted between the stem and the ergative/accusative.
  • The allative suffix always inserts a hiatus after a vowel.
  • Vowel harmony must be observed.

Tanmuran nouns do not properly decline; they are generally invariant with respect to case markers. However, there are exceptions (e.g. kami becomes kanna and mezu becomes mina in the genitive).

Pronouns work differently from nouns. A comparison of forms between the languages is listed below (using the first person singular pronoun):

Namari New Kudara Tanmuran
Nominative wae ware ware
Accusative wao wau
Genitive waga waga waga
Dative wan wez wen
Instrumental waji weš wasse
Comitative wato wato watto
Ablative 1 wai wōl weri
Ablative 2 wakaya wakara
Allative wampe wappe
Terminative wamade wamade wamade
Locative wande wad
Abessive wanku

The other personal pronouns decline identically. The demonstrative ka- declines similarly, except in the genitive, where the form depends on animacy (and in the New Kudara accusative, whose form is kaw).

The demonstrative ko-, which is more regular, declines as follows:

Namari New Kudara Tanmuran
Nominative koe kore kore
Accusative kou
Genitive kono kono kono
Dative kon koz kon
Instrumental koji koš kosse
Comitative koto koto kotto
Ablative 1 koyoi kojol kori
Ablative 2 kokaya kokara
Allative kompe koj koppe
Terminative komade komade komade
Locative konde kod
Abessive konku

The same applies to the other /o/ demonstratives and the interrogative (although vowel harmony applies in New Kudara).

The New Kudara pronoun šrī ("she") declines as follows:

Nominative šrī
Accusative šū
Genitive škœ
Dative šəz
Instrumental šiš
Comitative štū
Ablative šul
Allative šī
Terminative šmədi
Locative šəd

New Kudara is the only featured language to have gendered third-person pronouns; Namari shie is used regardless of gender or animacy, while Tanmuran kare applies to both males and females (inanimates use the general demonstrative sore; a word kanojo meaning "she" does exist in Tanmuran, but it is borrowed from Japanese and rarely used). The New Kudara gendered pronouns are kare ("he"), šrī ("she") and suri ("it").

Nominal adjectives

All three languages featured have nominal adjectives whose properties are separate from nouns. In all languages, nominal adjectives must be paired with the copula in order to act predicatively. When acting adnominally, they take the genitive. However, the form of the genitive does not necessarily match that of a normal noun, and varies between languages:

  • Namari nominal adjectives have their genitives influenced by a limited vowel harmony rule, with -na used when the word has /a/, /u/ or their derivatives, and -no otherwise.
  • New Kudara nominal adjectives have their genitives vary depending on the gender of the noun it modifies. The default is -na/-nə for the feminine, with the masculine being -ga/-gə and the neuter being -no/-nu.
  • Tanmuran nominal adjectives generally have their genitives in -na.

All nominal adjectives form their degree forms the same way as their verbal counterparts, and use the dative as adverbial forms. The comparative and superlative forms of the nominal adjectives are listed below:

Namari New Kudara Tanmuran
Comparative -moi -moram/-murəm
Superlative -motakakai -magesam/-məgisəm -nyōi

Other markers

All three languages feature a topic marker and a focus marker. The topic marker is *pa in Proto-Japonic, which is reflected in Namari -(h)a, New Kudara -a/ and Tanmuran -wa. I tentatively reconstruct the focus particle as *sə, and this is reflected in Namari -zu and -koso, New Kudara -zo/-zu and -koso/-kusu, and Tanmuran -zo and -koso. I cannot reconstruct any other focus markers at the moment, although additional markers do exist.

The focus marker has the property of changing the inflection of the main verb. In a normal declarative sentence, the main verb is in its conclusive form. However, the existence of a focus marker forces the main verb to take a different conjugation instead (in the case of -zo/-zu, it is the attributive, and in the case of -koso/-kusu, it is an emphatic form).

The New Kudara topic marker has special forms when combined with case markers (except the genitive, which cannot combine with the topic marker):

Proto-Japonic New Kudara
Ergative *i pa -ja/-jə/-i/-e
Intransitive *pa -a/-ə
Accusative *wo(n) pa -oba/-ubə
Dative *ni pa -za/-zə
Instrumental *si pa -ša/-šə
Comitative *tə pa -ta/-tə
Ablative *yo-ri pa -(j)ola/-(j)ulə
Allative *pe pa -ea/-iə
Terminative *man-te pa -maža/-məžə
Locative -da/-də

The pronominal nominative -re/-ri becomes -la/-lə when topicalised.

The New Kudara pronoun šrī ("she") has special topicalised forms:

Nominative šlœ
Accusative šubə
Dative
Instrumental šœ
Comitative štœ
Ablative šulə
Allative šijə
Terminative šməžə
Locative šnœ